IB Psychology.com

The Internal assessment

All about the internal assessment

Internal assessment checklist

Ethic checklist

 

 

The Internal assessment

The internal assessment is a key compnonent of the IB Psychology. In this practical you will replicate an actual psychology experiment.

Expectations

1) you may work by yourself, with a partner, or in a triple.

2) you MUST do you your own write-up. This is the only way to earn a grade.

3) Ethics considerations must be observed:

• Conformity studies are nearly all out - as are all animal experiments
• Any stereotyping practical to do with race, religion, gender or social class is out
• Quasi experiments are out - e.g. where the IV is age/race/gender/class/ability
• Informed consent must always be obtained, and debriefing must be carried out.

3) You must have it approved by me. Check out the list of possibilities.

4) Follow all the procedures outlined and meet deadlines.

 

Ideas for IB internal assessments

Don’t assume you can do the practical just because it appears here! It may be unethical or not an experiment or a quasi-experiment.

Please note that your study is supposed to be based on a previous experiment carried out by psychologists (i.e. a replication).

My Big List (read all to discover the many, many possibilities)

A MEMORY

B PERCEPTION, THINKING AND PERFORMANCE

C CONFORMITY AND PREJUDICE and other social topics

D DRIVES AND MOTIVATION

A MEMORY

1. Memory and the serial position curve: Subjects learn a long list of words and after a delay they have to recall as many as possible. The hypothesis is that people tend to remember the first and last words in a list due to the primacy and recency effects but not the middle words.
2. Memory and acoustic encoding:
1. There is a theory that when we have to learn something like a telephone number that we store the number in the form of the mental sound that it makes i.e. acoustically. Subjects have to learn lists of letters and then write them down after a delay. According to the acoustic coding theory subjects will have more difficulty recalling letters which sound similar compared to a list of words which sound quite different.
3. Improving memory:
1. Learning and recall can be helped by making associations with something else. Words to be learned are associated with other words which are used later to help retrieval.
2. Give subjects an ambiguous passage (which could mean anything) which they later have to recall. Some subjects are given a title to the passage which makes it sensible while other subjects are not. Those with the title should process the passage more meaningfully and therefore recall the passage more successfully.
3. Imagery vs rehearsal: participants recall more words from a (20) word list when they use an imagery method (forming a vivid mental image and linking each item to the last in a dynamic fashion) than if they use either rehearsal (repeat each item until you hear the next) or no particular method (no prior instruction). Bower (1967); Paivio (1971).
4. Organisation and memory: If word lists are organised in some meaningful way, subjects will recall better than from a jumbled list?
4. Memory and levels of processing: Craik and Lockhart hypothesise that the deeper and more meaningfully we process information the better subsequent recall will be. Subjects are asked to process words either at a basic structural level like 'is the word in capitals?' or at a level requiring the comprehension of meaning e.g. 'is it something you can eat'? Subjects would be expected to recall those words processed more deeply more successfully. Craik and Tulving (1975).
5. Memory interference: This could be nicely applied to school revision. Subjects have to learn for example a list of words and then recall them. However, memory is interfered with by learning another list of words but some subjects learn this interfering list before the main list and some learn it after the main list to see which has the greater effect.
6. Eye-witness reports: Loftus and Palmer (1974); Loftus and Zanni (1975). Participants asked how fast cars were going when they ‘smashed’ into each other, after viewing a car accident, report greater speeds than do participants asked the speed when they ‘hit’ each other. The former group are more likely to report seeing broken glass (when none is there) a week later. Participants are more likely to report seeing broken glass if the question uses ‘did you see the broken glass?’ rather than ‘....any broken glass?’. Weapon focus is linked. Loftus et al (1987) showed that when a person entered a room with a gun, syringe or cheque in their hand, participants recalled least features of the person (hair colour, facial characteristics) in the gun condition and most in the cheque condition. This could be replicated using pictures or video. Health and Safety brochures and videos are a useful resource for this.
7. Does time of day influence peoples ability to recall words?
8. Does background noise impair memory?

B PERCEPTION, THINKING AND PERFORMANCE

1. Perceptual Set:

1. This one is based on the idea that people have a very fixed association between colour and taste and attempts to find out how strong this is. It involves giving subjects coloured drinks but in some conditions the colour doesn't match up with the flavour (e.g. red coloured mint). By measuring how long it takes for subjects to judge the flavour will tell us how strong the associations are.
2. This is based on the hypothesis that peoples perception of colour depends on what they associate that colour with. For example people associate tomatoes with being red so might perceive a stronger red colour than say a red hat which doesn't have the same associations. This practical involves showing subjects pictures of fruit but with some of the colours mixed up. For example subjects are showed a picture of a red tomato and then a red banana and later have to judge the colours of each on a colour chart.
3. Solving lists of anagrams is easier if all the words belong to a category (e.g. animals) than if they are random words.
4. It has been suggested that people perceive the size of coins depending on how they perceive it's value. Specifically children and people from low income families are thought to overestimate the size of coins. This can be designed easily enough by showing subjects different coins and asking them to later to judge their size using a comparison chart of different sized circles.
5. This is based on the old/young woman ambiguous figure you may have seen - but will only work on students who haven't seen it (or you could invent your own alternative). The hypothesis is that if you predispose someone to think positively or even romantically by reading a short story or showing a film clip they will be more likely to see the young woman than the old witch.
6. People are first predisposed to think about a particular set of objects e.g. fruit or letters of the alphabet, by showing them pictures. Then an ambiguous picture is flashed to them which could be a banana or a letter 'C' for example. The hypothesis is that they will label the object according to the set of objects they saw previously.
7. The subject is presented briefly with a list of words about a topic e.g. letter, post, stamp etc. which they have to write down - and then one of them is misspelled e.g. mael, and the hypothesis is that because a strong mental concept of the topic has been set up that they will write down the word as 'mail'.
8. Illusions: This is based on the Muller-Lyer illusion: By re-designing the illusion it is possible to test the hypothesis of constancy scaling put forward by Richard Gregory that the reason the illusion works is because it has hidden depth cues. The practical involves showing subjects different forms of the illusion to find out when it is most powerful.

1. Stroop effect: Participants take a lot longer to name the colour of ink that words are written in when the words themselves are contradictory colour words e.g. ‘red’ written in yellow ink – Dyer (1973).
2. Word and letter recognition: Visual search: Time taken to find X’s hidden in a four column list of similar shaped letters (Y, Z etc.) is longer than for lists with letters such as S, R, P etc. – Neisser’s (1964) feature analysis model of pattern recognition. Alternatively: Participants will take longer to find 0 among letters if it is called tzero’ than when it is called letter ‘oh’ and vice versa – Jonides & Gleitman (1972).
3. Is performance is impeded by noise? Subjects have a task to perform with or without noise.
4. Estimation of time: Effects of mental activity on the estimation of time: Ornstein hypothesised that the more mental activity we do in a fixed period of time the longer the subjective estimate of that time interval. This could be tested by asking subjects to listen to a passage of prose for say 100 seconds and then to estimate when a further 100 seconds of the prose had elapsed. In one group the prose could be read slowly to reduce the amount of mental processing and in the second group the prose would be read out faster.
5. Concrete v abstract reasoning (problem solving) and concept formation: When it comes to logical problem solving people are typically very poor and tend just to look for examples which confirm their theory rather than looking for possible exceptions which might disprove it. This practical is based on giving subjects seemingly simple tests of logic to see what errors they make. However, the hypothesis being tested is that if the logic problems are purely abstract (e.g. if x is always double y and y is never an even number .....etc.) then many more mistakes will be made compared to more concrete problems (e.g., if worms are always animals and animals are sometimes birds....etc.) even if the basic logical pathway is the same. A similar alternative is as follows: The Wason (1969) selection task –participants are shown four cards displaying B E 3 and 8. They are given the rule ‘every consonant has an even number on the other side’. Participants have to decide which two cards to turn over in order to check the validity of the rule. They tend to demonstrate false logical reasoning and assume that the B and 8 cards must be turned over whereas B and 3 are correct. Griggs and Cox (1982) used more realistic material with ‘beer’ or ‘Pepsi’ on one side and an age on the other. To check the rule ‘only 18 year olds and above may drink’, and given cards showing ‘beer’, ‘Pepsi’, ‘16’ and ‘20’, participants do much better than on the abstract task. Similarly, participants might be asked to check the rule ‘all first class envelopes are sealed’ given sealed unsealed, first and second class stamped envelopes. Frequencies of correct response can be compared with those for the abstract Wason task using chi–squared.
6. Heuristics: Tversky and Kahneman’s (1973) ‘availability’ hypothesis. If people recall more items from one set than from another they assume (heuristically) that there actually were more in the former set. Demonstrate this by giving participants a set of names to remember containing 19 very famous males and 20 not so famous females. Since participants tend to recall more male names they tend to judge that more males were in the list.
7. Attitudes and classical conditioning: Involves making positive or negative associations between words to see if this changes our attitudes (see Flanagan)
8. First impressions and Primacy effects: ask participants to score a person’s maths test and then estimate the overall score on that person on Maths ability: Group A have correct answers first, then incorrect. Group B have incorrect answers first, then correct. Group A will estimate a higher score than Group B.

C CONFORMITY AND PREJUDICE and other social topics

1. Prejudice and stereotypes:

1. Subjects are presented with a list of facts about someone like 'tall', and 'keen on sport' etc. They then have to fill in a questionnaire about the personality traits they think that person would have. There are two experimental groups with identical lists of facts except for one key difference e.g. 'black'/'white' (this particular version is not ethical). This attempts to discover peoples prejudices about race, sex, social class etc. Social class stereotypes. Subjects hear identical lists of descriptions about a person except for him living in a large country house or a high rise flat. Subjects then fill in a questionnaire. Will need modifying and simplifying for IBs.
2. Person perception: Versions of Asch’s (1946) ‘warm’, ‘cold’ central traits paradigm can be implemented in many topical ways. Candidates give one description of a person to one group of participants and an identical version to another group varying only one characteristic, for instance ‘agrees with nuclear testing’ for one group and ‘disagrees....’ for the other. They then ask participants to assess the fictitious person on, say, liking or trustworthiness on a 10 point scale and look for differences between groups. Asch’s ‘primacy’ effect can also be tested using a list of descriptors – e.g. ‘orderly entertaining humble cool calculating moody’ in that order for one group and in the opposite order for another. Those hearing the positive traits first might rate the person more favourably on a ten point scale – Anderson and Barrios (1961).
3. Snyder and Uranowitz (1978) presented two versions of a woman’s life to two different groups. One group was later told that she took up a lesbian life-style (avoid this specific example); for the other group, she married. From the statement in the description ‘although she never had a steady boyfriend she did go out on dates’, the former group were more likely to recall the first part of this statement and the second group did the reverse.
4. Gordon et al (1988) showed that, given a scenario about a crime, black muggers and white embezzlers got heavier sentences than the opposite combinations, supporting the view that the crimes are stereotypical in people’s minds for the categories of person. Typically candidates can write a scenario and vary sex, ethnic group, occupation of the criminal or other fictitious actor and look for difference in how the fictitious person is sentenced, rated for liking or otherwise appropriately assessed. Care must be taken in such studies, however, not to reinforce any existing stereotypes which candidates already hold.
5. Luria and Rubin (1974) – participants given the same picture of a baby but one group told it is male the other female. Record differences in descriptions. It is best to give a checklist to participants containing ‘typical’ masculine and feminine traits – fine featured, strong, robust, sensitive, cute, delicate etc
6. The halo effect: The effects of physical attractiveness: The halo effect states that attractive people are perceived as having more positive attributes.

1. Person perception and first impressions: A two paragraph description or story of someone is written with one paragraph being more positive than the other. Paragraph order can be reversed and subjects get one of the two possible orders. Subjects then have to rate the person on a questionnaire. The hypothesis is that due to first impressions the subjects who hear the story with the first paragraph being the positive one will rate the person more positively. E.g. Story of Jim with the first half suggesting he is friendly and the second half suggestion he is unfriendly. Different subjects receive different orders of the story.
2. Social facilitation: The idea is that people tend to perform better when in groups than when on their own. Subjects can be given tasks (e.g. word searches) either in groups or on their own to test this theory.
3. Conformity: Do people over-estimate the number of beads in a jar if they see a list of other peoples ‘over-estimates’? i.e. do they conform to other peoples views?
4. Defensive attribution: The more serious an accident appears, the more people wish to assign responsibility to someone.

D DRIVES AND MOTIVATION

1. Incentives and performance: It would seem logical that incentives should improve our performance. This could be tested by asking subjects to perform simple tasks like anagrams and measuring their speed of performance under different conditions. e.g. with or without an incentive like a Mars bar or alternatively by creating a fear of failing - to see whether positive or negative incentives are the most effective (e.g. telling the subjects that the results will be put on display and that the average number of completed anagrams in 5 minutes by 8 year olds is 15!) This could be developed to find whether the fear of failure impedes performance most on more complex cognitive tasks like anagrams rather than simple memory recall type tasks. Alternatively you could test the hypothesis that a group of subjects will perform better on a task if they have previously (based on an earlier test) been told they have scored highly, than a group who have been told (falsely) they have performed badly.
2. Is performance in a task impeded by noise?

 

From the IB

Introduction
Cognitive psychology is concerned with how people acquire, store, transform, use and communicate information. Cognitive psychologists rejected the behaviourist assumption that mental events or states were unsuitable for scientific research.

In this unit students will examine the model-based approach often employed by the cognitive perspective. Issues of ecological and construct validity will be explored with regard to the methods employed by the perspective. Memory is the topic which we will study in depth.

Objectives of the Unit:
• Describe and evaluate the cultural context and development, the conceptual framework, the methodology, and the application of the cognitive perxpective.
• Describe and evaluate theories and empirical studies within this perspective.
• Explain how cultural, ethical, gender, and methodological considerations affect the interpretation of behaviour from a cognitive perspective.
• Compare theories, empirical studies and the conceptual framework of this model with the other perspectives.
• Identify and explain the strengths and limitations of cognitive explanations of behaviour.
• Explain the extent to which free will and determinism are integral in this perspective.
• Assess the extent to which concepts and models of information processing have helped the understanding of cognition.
• Assess claims that this perspective lacks ecological validity, and be able to consider alternative research methods.

.

A little humor

About Us | Site Map | Privacy Policy | Contact Us | ©2003 Shaun McElroy